Contributor: Daisuke Shinagawa
Vowel: /i, e, a, o, u/
Consonant:
Lexical contrast of tone is illustrated by the following near-minimal pair.
Tonal contour can only be realised in the vowel sequence /V.V/, which is phonologically analyzed as a disyllabic unit in this language, i.e., there is no empirical evidence that this language has a phonemic contour tone.
As shown in parameter 5, at least 4 distinctive levels of tonal height can be phonetically distinguished, i.e., in addition to [H] and [L] as a default realization of /H/ and /Ø/ respectively, upstepped H [ꜛH] and downstepped H [ꜜH] are identified.
However, these two tones seem not to be phonemic as their realization can appropriately be explained by tone rules. See notes in the parameter 5 for further description.
As in many Bantu languages, only /H/ can be a target of tone rules such as tonal shift and tonal reduction. In the following examples, the high tone underlyingly assigned to the 1PL object marker dú- is realised on the next TBU when followed by the toneless verb stem -loli- (tonal shift), whereas the same high tone is deleted when followed by a high-toned verb -káb- due to the restriction of high tone sequence (tonal reduction). On the other hand, no evidence that low tone is a target of any tonal rule is found so far, i.e., there is no empirical ground of a marked /L/ tone in this language.
As in many Bantu languages, only /H/ can be a target of tone rules such as tonal shift and tonal reduction. In the following examples, the high tone underlyingly assigned to the 1PL object marker dú- is realised on the next TBU when followed by the toneless verb stem -loli- (tonal shift), whereas the same high tone is deleted when followed by a high-toned verb -káb- due to the restriction of high tone sequence (tonal reduction). On the other hand, no evidence that low tone is a target of any tonal rule is found so far, i.e., there is no empirical ground of a marked /L/ tone in this language.
(3-1-1) | a. néŋ̍́lolja | vs. | b. nédulólja |
---|---|---|---|
ní=e-Ø-ŋ̍≠loli-a | ní=e-Ø-dú≠loli-a | ||
FOC=SM1-PRS-OM1≠see-FV | FOC=SM1-PRS-OM1PL≠see-FV | ||
‘S/he sees him/her’ | ‘S/he sees us’ |
(3-1-2) | a. néŋ̍́lolja | vs. | b. nédukabá |
---|---|---|---|
ní=e-Ø-ŋ̍≠káb-a | ní=e-Ø-dú≠káb-a | ||
FOC=SM1-PRS-OM1≠hit-F | FOC=SM1-PRS-OM1PL≠hit-F | ||
‘S/he hit him/her’ | ‘S/he hit us’ |
As shown in the notes on parameter 2, this language allows phonetic contour in the vowel sequence, e.g., CVV́Cː ikoóʈa ‘to draw’, and CV́VCː akáambá ‘and s/he said...’. However, as these vowel sequences can be phonologically analyzed as a disyllabic unit, the TBU of this language seems to be regarded as a syllable rather than a mora. This interpretation can be supported by the fact that when high tone shift occurs, an underlying H is regularly realised on one syllable right from the original locus, e.g., the final H tone on the verb akáambá is lexically assigned to the root -ámb- and the high tone realises on the next syllable, not on the next mora (i.e., *akáaḿba).
As provided in the notes on parameter 3, two allotones of phonemic H are identified in this language, namely upstepped H and downstepped H. Upstepped H is typically attested at the syntactic boundary between a verb and any preverbal element. Specifically, the focus marking high tone, which is associated to the initial TBU of the verb, is realised as an upstepped high when preceded by a preverbal constituent with a lexical high tone on its final syllable. On the other hand, the downstepped high is frequently observed in the NP domain, where underlying HH sequence at the phrase final position is regularly realised as HꜜH.
Upstep creationː H→ꜛH / H] VP[_
(5-1) | m̩-di ʃú | H=u-i-≠u-a |
3-tree DEM.N.3 | FOC=SM3-PROG≠fall-F | |
‘This tree is falling |
Downstep creation (MR): H→ꜜH / nominal stem[H_]#
(5-2) | ʃú | ni mwarílꜜé |
ʃú | ní m̩-árilé | |
DEM.N.3 COP 3-bow | ||
‘This is a bow’ |
Note: Please add examples wherever available
As shown in Table 6-1-1, all possible combinations of /H/ and /Ø/ except for /ØØ/ [LL] sequence are attested in disyllabic nominal stems.
However, in the trisyllabic stems, possible tonal patters are restricted in a way that surface realization of HH or LL sequence is basically disallowed (except for LHꜜH).
As in many Bantu languages, and in the reconstructed Proto Bantu lexicon, the contrast of lexical tone on the verb is manifested solely on the initial syllable of the stem. As shown in the Table 6-2-1, the lexical high tone is realised on the syllable next to the initial syllable, whereas a high tone assigned to the OM is realised on the initial syllable of a tone-less verb stem. It should also be noted that the tonal property of the verb seems to be at least partially relevant to the syllable structure of the stem in a way that no tone-less verb is attested in the class of -CVVC- stems.
Table 6-2-1. Tonal property of the verb stem and its syllable structure
n.a.
The 2SG OM and the reflexive marker are segmentally identical but tonally distinctive, i.e., 2SG ku- vs. REF kú-. Thus, the other things being equal, a pair of the same transitive verb with the 2SG OM and the reflexive marker is solely differentiated by tone.
n.a.
n.a.
Main clause negation is morphologically marked by the clause final negation particle kú, which is estimated to be cognate with the class 17 locative demonstrative pronoun. In addition to the particle, tonal modification of is also relevant to the main clause negation marking. As shown in (7-5), the initial high tone that is associated with sentence affirmation (tentatively glossed as FOC in (7-5a)) is absent in the negative form and the underlying high tone assigned to kú is spread left-ward to the TAM le- (if a stem has a lexical high tone, the spread ends in the nearest tone-less TBU, i.e., the second syllable of the stem).
As mentioned in the notes on parameter 5, the focus marking clitic ní= has a lexical high tone and the segmental ni= frequently drops, resulting that the focus, more precisely saying ‘main clause affirmation’ as part of truth-value focus, is solely marked by the initial high tone. In contrast, the lack of the initial high tone on the verb defectively signals that the form is a non-main clause verb. This is illustrated in (7-6-1), where the main clause verb dúlem̩lolia is realised with the initial high tone, whereas the relative verb eledusaidja lacks the high tone.
n.a. (so far)
Pitch track samples of the possible nominal tone patterns of trisyllabic words